Voiced velar approximant

Voiced velar approximant
ɰ
IPA Number154
Audio sample
source · help
Encoding
Entity (decimal)ɰ
Unicode (hex)U+0270
X-SAMPAM\
Braille⠦ (braille pattern dots-236)⠍ (braille pattern dots-134)

The voiced velar approximant is a type of consonantal sound, used in some spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is ⟨ɰ⟩, and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is M\.

The consonant is absent in English, but may be approximated by making but with the tongue body lowered or but with the lips apart. The voiced velar approximant can in many cases be considered the semivocalic counterpart of the close back unrounded vowel . ⟨ɰ⟩ and ⟨ɯ̯⟩ with the non-syllabic diacritic are used in different transcription systems to represent the same sound.

In some languages, such as Spanish, the voiced velar approximant is an allophone of /g/ – see below.

The symbol for the velar approximant originates from ⟨ɯ⟩, but with a vertical line. Compare ⟨u⟩ and ⟨ɥ⟩ for the labio-palatal approximant.

Features

Features of the voiced velar approximant:

Occurrence

Language Word IPA Meaning Notes
Aragonese caixigo 'oak tree' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; allophone of /ɡ/.
Astur-Leonese Asturian Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; allophone of /ɡ/.
Extremaduran
Leonese
Mirandese
Catalan aigua 'water' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; allophone of /ɡ/. See Catalan phonology
Cherokee ᏩᏥ wa-tsi 'watch' Found only in the Western dialect. Its equivalent in other dialects is . Also represented by Ꮺ, Ꮻ, Ꮼ, Ꮽ, and Ꮾ
Danish Older speakers talg 'tallow' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding. Still used by some older speakers in high register, much more commonly than a fricative . Depending on the environment, it corresponds to or in young speakers of contemporary Standard Danish. See Danish phonology
Dutch Western East Flemish Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding. Corresponds to a fricative in other dialects.
French Belgian ara 'macaw' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding. Intervocalic, word-internal allophone of /ʀ/ for some speakers. See French phonology
Galician auga 'water' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; allophone of /ɡ/. See Galician phonology
Greek Cypriot μαγαζί 'shop' Allophone of /ɣ/.
Guarani gotyo 'near, close to' Contrasts with
Ñandewa Paulista-Paranaense 'I cut' Contrasts with .
Hiw ter̄og 'peace' Contrasts with /w/ and with /ɡ͡ʟ/.
Ibibio ufokọ Intervocalic allophone of /k/; may be a uvular tap instead.
Icelandic saga 'saga' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding. See Icelandic phonology
Irish naoi 'nine' Occurs only between broad consonants and front vowels. See Irish phonology
Korean 의사 / uisa 'doctor' Occurs only before /i/. See Korean phonology
Mwotlap haghag 'sit' Contrasts with .
Shipibo igi Unspecified for rounding; varies between an approximant and a fricative. Allophone of /k/ in certain high-frequency morphemes.
Spanish pagar 'to pay' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; allophone of /ɡ/. See Spanish phonology
Swedish Central Standard agronom 'agronomist' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; allophone of /ɡ/ in casual speech. See Swedish phonology
Tagalog igriega 'y (letter)' Approximant consonant unspecified for rounding; intervocalic allophone of /ɡ/. See Tagalog phonology
Tiwi ngaga 'we (inclusive)'
Venetian góndola 'gondola' See Venetian language
Vietnamese Southern 'chicken' Typical realization of /ɡɣ/ or /ɣ/ in other dialects. Variant is in complementary distribution before open vowels.

Pre-velar

Voiced pre-velar approximant
ɰ˖
ɨ̯
Audio sample
source · help
Encoding
X-SAMPAj-
Language Word IPA Meaning Notes
Spanish seguir 'to follow' Approximant consonant. Lenited allophone of /ɡ/ before front vowels; typically transcribed in IPA with ⟨ɣ⟩. See Spanish phonology
Turkish Standard prescriptive düğün 'marriage' Either post-palatal or palatal; phonetic realization of /ɣ/ (also transcribed as /ɰ/) before front vowels. See Turkish phonology

Voiced velar bunched approximant

Voiced velar bunched approximant
ɹ̈
Encoding
X-SAMPAr\_"

Some languages have a velar approximant that is produced with the body of the tongue bunched up at the velum and simultaneous pharyngealization. This gives rise to a type of retroflex resonance resembling . The extension to the IPA recommends the use of the "centralized" diacritic combined with the IPA sign for the alveolar approximant (as in ⟨ɹ̈⟩) to distinguish the bunched realization from the prototypical apical , which may be specified as ⟨ɹ̺⟩. Typically, the diacritic is omitted, so that the sound is transcribed simply with ⟨ɹ⟩ or ⟨ɻ⟩ (in broader transcriptions: ⟨r⟩), as if it were a coronal consonant. Just as described below, the velar bunched approximant is not specified for rounding.

In Dutch, this type of r is called Gooise r 'Gooi r'. It is named after het Gooi, a region of the Netherlands where Hilversum (the main centre for television and radio broadcasting) is located.

Features

Features of the voiced velar bunched approximant:

Occurrence

Language Word IPA Meaning Notes
Dutch Randstad varieties maar 'but' Pre-velar. Common allophone of /r/ in the syllable coda, where it contrasts with . The bunching and pharyngealization may be lost in connected speech, resulting in a semivowel such as or . See Dutch phonology
Standard Northern Pre-velar. Common allophone of /r/ in the syllable coda, where it contrasts with . See Dutch phonology
English American red 'red' Labialized approximant consonant. Possible realization of /r/. Contrasts with /w/. See Pronunciation of English /r/
Received Pronunciation curious 'curious' Approximant consonant, may be labialized. Possible allophone of /r/ before front vowels. Contrasts with /w/. See Pronunciation of English /r/

Relation with and

Some languages have a voiced velar approximant that is unspecified for rounding, and therefore cannot be considered the semivocalic equivalent of either or its rounded counterpart . Examples of such languages are Catalan, Galician and Spanish, in which the approximant consonant (not semivowel) unspecified for rounding appears as an allophone of /ɡ/.

Eugenio Martínez Celdrán describes the voiced velar approximant consonant as follows:

As for the symbol ⟨ɰ⟩, it is quite evidently inappropriate for representing the Spanish voiced velar approximant consonant. Many authors have pointed out the fact that is not rounded; for example, Pullum & Ladusaw (1986:98) state that 'the sound in question can be described as a semi-vowel (glide) with the properties "high", "back", and "unrounded"'. They even establish an interesting parallelism: 'the sound can be regarded as an unrounded '. It is evident, then, that ⟨ɰ⟩ is not an adequate symbol for Spanish. First of all, because it has never been taken into consideration that there is a diphthong in words like paga 'pay', vago 'lazy', lego 'lay', etc., and, secondly, because this sound is rounded when it precedes rounded vowels. Besides, it would be utterly wrong to transcribe the word jugo 'juice' with ⟨ɰ⟩ *, because the pronunciation of that consonant between two rounded vowels is completely rounded whereas is not.

The symbol I have always proposed is ⟨ɣ̞⟩, the correlate to the other central approximants in Spanish, (Martínez Celdrán 1991, 1996:47). This coincides with Ball & Rahilly (1999:90), whose example for the three approximants is the Spanish word abogado 'lawyer'. Ball & Rahilly too criticise in a footnote the confusion between these symbols: 'The difference between an approximant version of the voiced velar fricative , and the velar semi-vowel is that the latter requires spread lips, and must have a slightly more open articulatory channel so that it becomes if prolonged' (p. 189, fn. 1).

There is a parallel problem with transcribing the palatal approximant.

The symbol ⟨ɣ̞⟩ may not display properly in all browsers. In that case, ⟨ɣ˕⟩ should be substituted. In broader transcriptions, the lowering diacritic may be omitted altogether, so that the symbol is rendered ⟨ɣ⟩, i.e. as if it represented the corresponding fricative.

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Mott (2007), pp. 104–105.
  2. ^ a b Carbonell & Llisterri (1992), p. 55.
  3. ^ a b Martínez Celdrán (2004), p. 204.
  4. ^ a b Grønnum (2005), p. 123.
  5. ^ Basbøll (2005), pp. 211–212.
  6. ^ a b Taeldeman (1979).
  7. ^ a b Demolin (2001), pp. 65, 71.
  8. ^ a b c Martínez Celdrán (2004), pp. 203–204.
  9. ^ Arvaniti (1999), p. 174.
  10. ^ a b Costa (2012), p. 78.
  11. ^ François (2010), pp. 397–400.
  12. ^ a b Urua (2004), p. 106.
  13. ^ François (2001), p. 60.
  14. ^ a b Valenzuela, Márquez Pinedo & Maddieson (2001), p. 282.
  15. ^ a b Martínez Celdrán (2004), pp. 202–204.
  16. ^ Engstrand (2004), p. 167.
  17. ^ a b Canellada & Madsen (1987), p. 21.
  18. ^ a b Zimmer & Orgun (1999), p. 155.
  19. ^ a b c d Collins & Mees (2003), p. 200.
  20. ^ Collins & Mees (2003), pp. 200–1.
  21. ^ a b c d Cruttenden (2014), pp. 225–6.
  22. ^ Martínez Celdrán (2004), pp. 202–203.
  23. ^ See e.g. Carbonell & Llisterri (1992).

References

External links